EVE198 / EVE161 Microbial Phylogenomics Course Spring 2012

Image

EVE 198 – Spring Quarter 2012 – CRN 74485 – 3 units P/NP grading

Microbial Phylogenomics:  The Evolutionary Diversification of Microbes and their Genomes

Professor Jonathan Eisen

 

 

TR 1:40-3:00pm

Room 1344 Storer Hall

 Prerequisites for this EVE 198 / Jonathan Eisen course are BIS 2ABC (or its equivalent, for transfer students). (This course is moving through the course approval process, and will eventually be EVE 161!)

New openaccess paper from my lab on "Zorro" software for automated masking of sequence alignments

A new Open Access paper from my lab was just published in PLoS One: Accounting For Alignment Uncertainty in Phylogenomics. Wu M, Chatterji S, Eisen JA (2012) Accounting For Alignment Uncertainty in Phylogenomics. PLoS ONE 7(1): e30288. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0030288

The paper describes the software “Zorro” which is used for automated “masking” of sequence alignments.  Basically, if you have a multiple sequence alignment you would like to use to infer a phylogenetic tree, in some cases it is desirable to block out regions of the alignment that are not reliable.  This blocking is called “masking.”

Masking is thought by many to be important because sequence alignments are in essence a hypothesis about the common ancestry of specific residues in different genes/proteins/regions of the genome.  This “positional homology” is not always easy to assign and for regions where positional homology is ambiguous it may be better to ignore such regions when inferring phylogenetic trees from alignments.

Historically, masking has been done by hand/eye looking for columns in a multiple sequence alignment that seem to have issues and then either eliminating those columns or giving them a lower weight and using a weighting scheme in the phylogenetic analysis.

What Zorro does is it removes much of the subjectivity of this process and generates automated masking patterns for sequence alignments.  It does this by assigning confidence scores to each column in a multiple seqeunce alignment. These scores can then be used to account for alignment accuracy in phylogenetic inference pipelines.

The software is available at Sourceforge: ZORRO – probabilistic masking for phylogenetics.  It was written primarily by Martin Wu (who is now a Professor at the University of Virginia) and Sourav Chatterji with a little help here and there from Aaron Darling I think.  The development of Zorro was part of my “iSEEM” project that was supported by the Gordon and Betty Moore Foundation.

In the interest of sharing, since the paper is fully open access, I am posting it here below the fold. UPDATE 2/9 – decided to remove this since it got in the way of getting to the comments …

Draft post cleanup #21: Tracking progress on the vertebrate tree of life

Yet another post in my “draft blog post cleanup” series. Here is #21; from March 2010:

A very interesting paper came out recently from colleagues of mine at UC Davis:  Rapid progress on the vertebrate tree of life.  I did not know they were working on this but perhaps should have.  It has some fun/interesting analysis of the accumulation of phylogenetic knowledge over time.  For example see Figure 1

Cumulative phylogenetic information amassed for the last 16 years. The accumulation of sequences for vertebrates in GenBank (a), papers using the term ‘phylogeny’ or ‘phylogenetics’ in the Web of Science database (b) and phylogenetic resolution (measured as the proportion of nodes with at least 50% bootstrap support) in the vertebrate tree of life resulting from these research efforts (c). In all cases, the data are cumulative from the start of each analysis. Phylogenetic resolution is calculated as in Table 1. Trend lines are exponential in (a), and second order polynomial in (b) and (c).

The rest of the paper is worth a look.

And alas I stopped there … I think I wanted to get Brad Shaffer and Bob Thomson’s comments on the paper but never got around to it.  Two years later the paper still is worth a look …

Very nice new #PLoSGenetics paper on "Functional Phylogenomics" of Seed Plants

Update2 – 12/22 – Data available here.  Thanks to the authors for clearing things up quickly.


Update1 –  12/19 – Data for this paper seems to be unavailable – not sure why – but looking into this after a TWEET from Karen Cranston. The paper says data is available at: http://nypg.bio.nyu.edu/main/ but I could not find any there.  Note – this is one reason that all data sets should be made available at the journal or third party sites.


Original post:

OK never mind that the terminology of “functional phylogenomics” is a tiny bit vexing to me (long story – some other time perhaps). The paper behind it – PLoS Genetics: A Functional Phylogenomic View of the Seed Plants is very cool.

Here’s what the authors did (a very coarse summary)

1. Identified sets of orthologs between plant species using the OrthologID system (which has a phylogenetic underpinning) (the data input for this appeared to have mostly been Unigene EST clusters)

2. Constructed a “total evidence” phylogeny for these taxa (using a few approaches) 

3. Use this phylogeny to reinterpret some general features of the evolution of plants 
4. Searched for gene ontology categories (in annotated genes from these organisms) that agreed with the phylogeny. In essence, this seems to be a search for shared-derived traits (i.e., synapomorphies) in particular clades. 
5. Generated hypothesis about functional evolution in particular clades.
Overall, there is a lot that is really fascinating in here and this approach seems very powerful (though I note – I think something akin to this though not as comprehensive or as careful has been done for other groups but not sure).  Check out the paper for more detail …
Lee EK, Cibrian-Jaramillo A, Kolokotronis S-O, Katari MS, Stamatakis A, et al. (2011) A Functional Phylogenomic View of the Seed Plants. PLoS Genet 7(12): e1002411. doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.1002411

Special Guest Post & Discussion Invitation from Matthew Hahn on Ortholog Conjecture Paper


I am very excited about today’s post.  It is the first in what I hope will be many – posts from authors of interesting papers describing the “Story behind the paper“.  I write extensive detailed posts about my papers and also have tried to interview others about their papers if they are relevant to this blog.  But Matthew Hahn approached me recently about the possibility of him writing up some details on his recent paper on the functions of orthologs vs. paralogs.  So I said “sure” and set up a guest account for him to write up his comments and details of the paper.  


For those of you who do not know, Matt is on the faculty at U. Indiana.  He was a post doc at UC Davis so I have a particular bias in favor of him.  But his recent paper has generated some controversy (I posted some links about it here).  So it is great to get some more detail from him.  In addition, I note, I am also using this approach to try and teach people how easy it is to write a blog post by getting them guest accounts on Blogger and letting them write up something with links, pictures, etc.  So hopefully we can get more scientists blogging too.


Anyway – without any further ado – here is Matt’s post:

———————————————————————–
Following Jonathan’s excellent example of how explaining the history of a project helps to illuminate how the process of science actually happens, I thought I’d start by giving a bit of history behind our study, and the paper that we recently published in PLoS Computational Biology (http://www.ploscompbiol.org/article/info:doi/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002073). And then I’ll address the critics…
How this all got started
It all started a bit more than three years ago, in the summer of 2008. Pedja (as Predrag Radivojac is known to friends) was giving a talk to a group of us on protein function prediction that he also presented as a tutorial at the Automated Function Prediction SIG at ISMB 2008. Pedja and I had already collaborated on a small project involving the evolution of phosphoryation sites, but I really had no idea about his work on function prediction, and little idea in general about how function prediction was done. Reviewing different ways to accomplish transfer-by-similarity, he eventually got around to evolutionary (phylogenomic) approaches. Here is what I remember of this specific exchange during his talk:
Pedja: …and of course these methods only use orthologs for prediction, because orthologs have more similar functions than do paralogs.
me (from audience): Who says?
Pedja: Umm, you say. I mean, the evolutionary biologists say.
me: No, we don’t. I don’t know of any data that says any such thing.
Pedja: Whatever, Matt. We’ll talk about this later.
Well, we did talk about it later, and it turned out that although this claim is made in tons of papers, there is basically no data to support it. In the best cases a real example of one gene family will be cited, but there are very few of these. In the worst cases, the authors will just cite some random paper about gene duplicates (or Fitch’s original paper defining orthologs and paralogs). Of course I agree that patterns of sequence evolution might lead you to conclude this relationship was true, but there was no experimental data.


In fact, as we say in our paper, rarely did anyone recognize that this claim needed to be tested, or even that it was a claim that could be tested. At the time Eugene Koonin was the only person to say this: “The validity of the conjecture on functional equivalency of orthologs is crucial for reliable annotation of newly sequenced genomes and, more generally, for the progress of functional genomics. The huge majority of genes in the sequenced genomes will never be studied experimentally, so for most genomes transfer of functional information between orthologs is the only means of detailed functional characterization” (http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/16285863). I really liked the way that Eugene had said this, and started to refer to the idea that orthologs were more functionally similar than paralogs as the “ortholog conjecture.” So to be clear: I completely made up this phrase, but used the most evocative word from the Koonin paper.
Luckily for Pedja and me we had just gotten a small internal grant to work on genome annotation and we had an incoming master’s student (Nathan Nehrt) who was willing to work on a project intending to test the ortholog conjecture.
Interlude: the crappy state of things in the study of the evolution of function
In order to test anything about how function evolves between orthologs and paralogs—or between any genes—one of course needs some kind of data on gene function in multiple species. And this turns out to be a big problem.
Because, as Koonin says in the earlier quote, the vast majority of experimental data comes from a very few species, and these species are not exactly closely related. Here is an approximate phylogeny of the major eukaryotic model organisms:
It’s obvious from this figure that if you need both 1) lots of functional data from two species, and 2) a pretty good idea of exactly what the homologous relationships are between the genes you’re studying, you’re going to have to study human and mouse.
This is actually a pretty bleak picture for people who study molecular evolution (as I do). While we have tons and tons of sequence data both within and between species, and a very good idea about how these sequences evolve, and fancy models with which to analyze these sequences…we know next to diddly-squat about general patterns relating these sequence differences to functional differences. There are lots of interesting things to be gleaned from studies of sequence evolution, but it really would be nice to know something about the relationship between sequence and function.
What we found
What exactly does the ortholog conjecture predict? In my mind, at least, it predicts something like this:
In this completely fictitious graph the relationship between protein function and sequence similarity is a declining one, only it declines faster for paralogs than it does for orthologs. Also, just possibly, gene duplicates start out with slightly diverged function the minute they appear. Anyway, those were our predictions.
But here is what we found (Figure 1 in Nehrt et al. 2011):

(Panel A uses the Biological Process ontology and panel B uses the Molecular Function ontology.)
There are really two different, equally surprising results here. First, there is no relationship between sequence divergence and functional divergence for orthologs (among 2,579 one-to-one orthologs between human and mouse). Absolutely none—it’s a straight horizontal line. Second, there is a relationship for paralogs (among 21,771 comparisons), exactly as we predicted there would be. So according to our results, paralogs actually have more conserved function than do orthologs. Our interpretation of the data was that the most important determinant of function was the organismal context in which a gene/protein found itself: given the same amount of sequence divergence, two proteins in the same organism would be more functionally similar. For orthologs, this means that the sequence divergence of our target gene was not the most important thing, but rather the sum total of divergence in all of the genes that contribute to its cellular context. Which is why all the orthologs have on average similar functional divergence—they are all exactly the same age and hence have approximately the same levels of divergence in these interactors (in this case sequence divergence for paralogs is a much better indicator of their splitting time).
Without going through every result in the paper and our interpretation of every result, suffice it to say that after about a year-and-a-half of working on this (around February 2010), we were satisfied that we had something we were willing to submit. I even seem to remember showing the above figure to Jonathan on a visit to UC-Davis! So we did submit the paper, first to PNAS and then, after rejection, to PLoS Computational Biology, where it was rejected again.
The content of the reviews was approximately the same at both journals. Basically, people were not convinced of our results mostly because the functional relationships were all based on data in the Gene Ontology database. To be specific, the functional data we used came from experiments conducted in 12,204 different papers. We didn’t use any predicted functions, only functions assigned using experimental data. And we did A LOT of work to try to eliminate problems that might have affected our results, including repeating the main analysis using only GO terms common to both the human and mouse datasets. But there can still be bias hidden within these functional assignments because someone always has to interpret the experiment—to say that a yeast two-hybrid experiment means that a gene has function X. And because of these biases, people weren’t buying it.
To get a measure of functional similarity that did not depend on the interpretation of any experiments, we decided to repeat the entire analysis using microarray data, using the correlation in expression levels across 25 tissues as the measure of functional similarity. By this time Nathan was graduating and moving on to Maricel Kann’s lab as a research programmer, so we recruited one of Pedja’s Ph.D. students, Wyatt Clark, to pick up where Nathan had left off. (Wyatt had actually been a student in my undergraduate Evolution course a few years earlier, so we figured he knew something…) After repeating all of the GO-based analyses himself—always better to double-check, right?—Wyatt got all of the microarray data in order and produced this figure (Figure 4 in Nehrt et al. 2011):
So a year after we first submitted a paper, we submitted a new version to PLoS CB including the array analysis, and this was enough to convince the reviewers that our results were not merely due to some strange bias in GO.
The fallout, and some responses
First, let me say that I had some idea that this would be a controversial-ish paper, and that we’d get at least some blowback. For about the first 20 versions of the manuscript (including some submitted versions) I put the words “ortholog conjecture” in quotes in the title, never an endearing move. (Pedja finally convinced me to take them out of the latest submissions.) But I also thought people would be happier that an untested assumption had finally been tested—and we have definitely gotten some positive feedback along these lines, including several groups that told us they have data that support our findings. By coincidence my lab had another paper come out the same week as this one (http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21636278), and I mistakenly thought it would generate much more attention. I still think the biological importance of the results in that one are much greater than the ortholog conjecture results, but either because we didn’t publish in an open-access journal (Jonathan is always right) or simply because the function-prediction community is more active on the interweb tubes, there have been a surprising number of critical responses (partially collected here: http://phylogenomics.blogspot.com/2011/09/some-links-on-ortholog-conjecture-paper.html). So here are some responses to general critiques.
The ortholog conjecture says only that orthologs are similar.
Okay, this one is a bit unfair, as only one person has said this. The real problem here is that Michael Galperin seems to have deeply misunderstood what we mean by the ortholog conjecture. According to him the ortholog conjecture is “the assumption that orthologs (genes with a common origin that were vertically inherited from the same gene in the last common ancestor of the host organisms) typically retain the same function or have closely related ones.” Umm, no. In fact, if you really think this is what the ortholog conjecture says, then our results support it—human and mouse orthologs do typically have closely related functions. But we are explicitly testing for a difference between orthologs and paralogs, not whether or not orthologs retain any functions. At no point did we say (or even hint) that orthologs should not be used for functional prediction. The whole point of our analysis and conclusions is that we should stop ignoring paralogs, which would give us a ton more data to use for the prediction of functions.
The assignments of orthology and paralogy are incorrect.
This is an easy one: we did in fact get the definitions of in- and out-paralogs correct (and laid them out in Figure S1). According to Sonnhammer and Koonin: “Our definition of ‘outparalogs’ is: paralogs in the given lineage that evolved by gene duplications that happened before the radiation (speciation) event” (http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12446146). For the purposes of our study, this means that outparalogs are defined as any paralogs that diverged before the speciation event between human and mouse and inparalogs diverged after this speciation event. Outparalogs do not indicate only paralogs in two different species, though by necessity in our dataset inparalogs are only found in the same species (all in human or all in mouse). Therefore, with respect to our conclusion that the most important determinant of function is which genome you are found in (i.e. context), it wouldn’t matter if we had incorrect gene trees: we would never confuse two genes in the same species (either inparalogs or some of the outparalogs) with two genes in different species (all orthologs and the remaining outparalogs).
You should have inferred functions yourselves
This is a fair suggestion, and not having enough time to annotate functions for 40,000 proteins would be a pretty weak excuse for doing good science. Instead…I’ll just say that it turns out professional curators are much better at assigning functions than even the original study authors (see http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/20829821). Curators have a much broader view of the whole set of terms available in any ontology, and a much more consistent idea of how to apply these terms. My favorite line from the above cited article: “…because of the relatively low accuracy of the authors’ submissions, the use of authors’ annotations did not result in saving of curators’ time…”
GO is not appropriate for this analysis because it is biased.
This is the most frustrating criticism of our study, if only because it’s partly true: GO is biased. In our paper we actually detail several of these biases, including the observation that the same set of authors will give two proteins more-similar functions than will two different sets of authors. We tried very hard to attempt to control for these biases, though of course one cannot account for all of them. The most uncharitable part of this critique, however, has to be the fact that people conveniently forgot to say that our array analysis was completely distinct from the GO-based analysis (though it has its own issues), and that Burkhard Rost’s analysis of protein-protein interaction (http://www.ploscompbiol.org/article/info:doi/10.1371/journal.pcbi.0020079) was also completely free of any bias in GO and was consistent with all of our results.
More annoying than this, you’d think from some of the critiques of GO that it was some sort of fly-by-night operation that no one should ever depend on. I mean, c’mon—there are human curators and human experimenters and of course they’re all biased so badly one could never compare functions between proteins much less between species. What were we thinking? (Only that the original GO paper has been cited >7000 times.) Funnily enough, at several points during the course of this work Pedja suggested—only half-jokingly—that we should just assume the ortholog conjecture was correct and write a paper about how GO must be wrong. Seriously, though: one would think from the excuses people came up with for the problems inherent in GO that we should simply stop using it to, you know, predict function in other species. And we were applying it to two relatively closely related mammals, one of which is explicitly a model for the other.
What next?
Our paper laid out several explicit hypotheses about the evolution of function that arose from our findings. Unfortunately, testing any of these hypotheses will require a ton more functional data, in more than one species. I know there are multiple groups working to collect these sorts of labor-intensive datasets, and Pedja and I are thinking about doing it ourselves (with collaborators, of course!). Massive datasets that reveal protein function will always be a lot harder to collect than sequence data, especially ones free from biases.
So let’s get to it…

—————————

Note – Toni Gabaldón was trying to post a detailed response but Blogger kept cutting him off with a character limit.  So I have posted his response below.

I appreciate the effort by Matthew Hahnn on explaining the story behind his paper on the so-called “Ortholog conjecture” and on facing some of the criticism. This paper attracted my interest as that of many others that work on or just use orthology. For instance it was chosen by one of my postdocs for our “Journal Club” meeting. And it was discussed during our last “Quest for Orthologs” meeting in Cambridge. I think is raising a necessary discussion and therefore I think is a good paper. This does not mean that I fully agree with the interpretation and conclusions ;-). I hope to modestly contribute to this debate with the following post.

I think one of the causes that this paper has caused so much debate is that the conclusions seem to challenge common practice (inferring function from orthologs), and could be interpreted as the need of changing the strategies of genome annotation. I think, however, that one should interpret carefully these results before start annotating based on paralogous proteins. As I will discuss below one of the problems is that we need to agree in what is the conjecture to then agree in how to test it. I see three main points that can be a source of confusion: i) the issue of what is actually stated by this conjecture, ii) the issue of annotation, and iii) the issue of time

1) What is the “ortholog conjecture”?
Or in other terms, when should we expect orthologs to be more likely to share function than paralogs?. Always? Of course not. All of us would agree that two recently duplicated paralogs are likely to be more similar in function than two distant orthologs, so it is obvious that the conjecture is not simply “orthologs are more similar in function than paralogs”. In reality the expectation that orthologs are more likely to be similar in function than paralogs, as least this is how I interpret it, is directly related to the effect that duplication have on functional divergence. If gene duplication has some effect on functional divergence (even in not 100% of the cases), then, given all other things equal (divergence time, story of speciation/duplication events – except fpr the duplication defining the orthologs) one would expect orthologs to be more likely to conserve function.

I think this complexity is not well considered (by many authors, in general). Hahn refeers to the famous review of orthology by Koonin (2005) as the source for the term “ortholog conjecture”. However, In that paper this conjecture is discussed always within the context of genes accross two particular species, whether in Hahn’s paper it is taken as well to other contexts. Thus, the proper context in which to test this conjecture is only between orthologs and between-species paralogs. As we can see,  Red and purple lines in Hahn paper in figure2 do not show any clear difference.

 Secondly, Koonin was very cautions in his paper, stating that he was referring to “equivalent functions” and not exactly the same “function”, correctly implying that the functional contexts would be different in the two different species. This brings me to the next point.

ii) annotation
If the expectation of functional conservation of orthologs refers to a given pair of species, then it makes no sense to test that expectation between paralogs within the same species and orthologs in different species. We were interested in this issue and it took us some effort to control for this “species” influence on the comparison, if you are interested you can read our paper on divergence of expression profiles between orthologs and paralogs (http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21515902)

As Hahn founds, and it was anticipated by Koonin in that review, there is a huge influence of the “species context”, a big constraint of what fraction of the function is shared. Indeed I think is the dominant signal in Hahn’s paper. Why is that? One possibility is that the functional context determines the function, I agree. However, we should not discard biases in how different communities working around a model species define processes and function, also the type of experiments that are usually done. For instance experimental inference from KO mutants might be common from mouse, but I guess is not the case in humans (!!). I think this may be having a big influence and might even be the dominant signal in Hahns paper.

Finally function has many levels and I expect subfunctionalization mostly affect lower levels (i.e. more specific). Biases may also
 exist in the level of annotation between species or between families of different size (contributing more or less to the orthologs/paralogs class).

Microarray data are less likely to be subject to biases (although some may exist), at least they should be expected to be free of “human interpretation biases” and so Hahn and colleaguies did well, in my opinion, of testing that dataset. It is important to note that for microarrays and for orthologs and between-species paralogs (which I think is the right frame for testing the conjecture) ortholgs are more likely to share an expression context. This is compatible to what we found in the paper mentioned above, and compatible with the orthology conjecture as stated by koonin (accross species)

iii) time
 Finally, one aspect which I think is fundamental is the notion of “divergence time”. Since paralogs can emerge at different time-scales they are composed by a heterogeneous set of protein pairs. Most of comparisons of orthologs and paralogs (Hahn’s as well) use sequence divergence as a proxy of time. However this is only a poor estimate, specially when duplications (as in here) are involved (we explored this issue in the past: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/21075746). This means that for a given divergence time paralogs may have larger sequence divergence than orthologs at the same divergence time, or otherwise (if gene conversion is playing a role). Is the conjecture based on sequence divergence or on divergence time?, I think the initial sense of using orthology to annotate accross species is based on the notion of comparing things at the same evolutionary distance. Thus basing our conclusions on divergence times might not be the proper way of doing it.

CONCLUSIONS AND PROPOSAL FOR RE-STATEMENT

To conclude, and with the intention of going beyond this particular paper,
I would finish by saying that the key to the problem lies on how we interpret the so-called “ortholog conjecture” or how are our expectations on how function evolves. What I get from re-reading Eugene Koonin’s paper and how I am using that “assumption” in my day-to-day work is the following:

“Orthologs in two given species are more likely to share equivalent functions than paralogs between these two species”

Therefore the notion of “accross the same pair of species” is important and thus only part of the comparisons made by Hahn and colleagues could directly test this. Looking at the microarray and between-species comparisons data, the conjecture may even hold true!!

I, however, do think that the conjecture as stated above is limited and does not capture the complexity of orthology relationships. Indeed us, and many other researchers, are tuning the confidence of the orthology-based annotation based on whether the orthologs are one-to-one, one-to-many or many-to-many, even when orthologs are “super-orthologs” (with no duplication event in the lineages separating the two orthologs).

Since, the underlying assumption of the ortholog conjecture is that duplication may (not necessarily always) promote functional shifts, then many-to-many orthology relationships will tend to include  orthologous pairs with different functions.

 Thus I would re-state the conjecture (or expectation) as follows:

 “In the absence of additional duplication events in the lineages separating them, two orthologous genes from two given species are more likely to share equivalent functions than two paralogs between these two species”

 This would be a more conservative expectation, which is closer to the current use of orthology-based annotation that tends to identify one-to-one orthologs, rather than any type.

 When duplications start appearing in subsequent lineages thus creating one- or many-to-many orthology relationships, the situation is less clear. Following the assumption that duplications may promote functional divergence. Then one could expand the conjecture by “the more duplications in the evolutionary history separating two genes, the lower the expectation that these two genes would share equivalent functions”.

 I wrote this contribution on the fly, and surely there are ways of expressing this in more appropriate terms. In any case I hope I made clear the idea that the conjecture emerges from the notion of duplications causing functional shifts and that our expectations will be clearer if expressed on those terms. This goes on the lines of what Jonathan Eisen mentioned on considering the whole phylogenetic story to annotate genes.

 Under this perspective, the real important hypothesis is that “duplications tend promote functional shifts”, I think this is based on solid grounds and has been tested intensively in the past.

 Cheers,

Toni Gabaldón

http://treevolution.blogspot.com

More on ‘phylogenomics’ – as in functional prediction w/ phylogeny

There is a new paper out: Phylogenetic-based propagation of functional annotations within the Gene Ontology consortium in Briefings in Bioinformatics.

The paper is interesting and presents a new general approach to using phylogeny for functional prediction of uncharacterized genes. I am interested in this for many reasons including that I was one of, if not the first to lay this out as a concept.  In a series of papers from 1995-1998 I outlined how phylogenetic analysis could be used to aid in functional prediction for all the genes that were starting to be sequenced in genome projects without any associated functional studies (at the time, I referred to all these ESTs and other sequences as an “onslaught” – little did I know what was to come).

My first paper on this topic was in 1995: Evolution of the SNF2 family of proteins: subfamilies with distinct sequences and functions.  The abstract is below:

The SNF2 family of proteins includes representatives from a variety of species with roles in cellular processes such as transcriptional regulation (e.g. MOT1, SNF2 and BRM), maintenance of chromosome stability during mitosis (e.g. lodestar) and various aspects of processing of DNA damage, including nucleotide excision repair (e.g. RAD16 and ERCC6), recombinational pathways (e.g. RAD54) and post-replication daughter strand gap repair (e.g. RAD5). This family also includes many proteins with no known function. To better characterize this family of proteins we have used molecular phylogenetic techniques to infer evolutionary relationships among the family members. We have divided the SNF2 family into multiple subfamilies, each of which represents what we propose to be a functionally and evolutionarily distinct group. We have then used the subfamily structure to predict the functions of some of the uncharacterized proteins in the SNF2 family. We discuss possible implications of this evolutionary analysis on the general properties and evolution of the SNF2 family.



I note – I am annoyed that when I went to the Nucleic Acids Research site for my paper I discovered for some bizarre reason they are now trying to charge for access to it even though it is in Pubmed Central and used to be freely available on the NAR site.  WTF?  Is this just an IT issue like the #OpenGate complaints I made for a while about Nature Genome papers.

Anyway – in that paper in 1995 I basically showed that at least for this family, phylogenetic analysis could be used as a tool in making functional predictions by allowing one to better identify orthology relationships and subfamilies within the SNF2 superfamily.  This was novel I think maybe a little bit but others at the time were also looking into using various analyses to identify orthology relationships across genomes.

Shortly thereafter I started working on the concept that one could used the phylogenetic tree more explicitly in making functional predictions and eventually I laid out the concept of treating function as a character states and doing character state reconstruction using a gene tree to then infer functions for uncharacterized genes.  I called this approach “phylogenomics” in a paper in 1997 in Nature Medicine (the editor asked us to give it a name … and thus my own contribution to the omics word game began).  Alas somehow the title of our paper became “Gatrogenomic delights” a movable feast” since we were writing about the E. coli and H. pylori genomes, so I added yet another omics term at the same time.  In the paper I showed how phylogenetic analysis of the MutS family of proteins could help in interpreting one of the findings in the H. pylori genome paper:

In this paper we showed why blast searches were not ideal for inferring relationships among sequences (because blast measures similarity NOT evolutionary history per se).  A bit annoyed still that other papers then sort of claimed they were the first to show blast was not ideal for inferring evolutionary relatedness, but whatever. This still did not fully describe the phylogeny driven approach that I was working on so I then wrote up an outline of this approach for a paper in Genome Research: Phylogenomics: Improving Functional Prediction for Uncharacterized Genes by Evolutionary Analysis.  This paper really laid out the idea in more detail:

It also gave detailed examples of how similarity searches could be misleading and how phylogenetic analysis should in principle be better.

I note – I am very very proud of this paper.  But it did not do a lot of things.  Really it was about laying out a concept of using tools from phylogenetics in functional prediction.  But it did not provide software for example.  I later developed some of my own scripts for doing this when I was at TIGR but really the software for phylogeny driven functional predictions would come later from others like Kimmen Sjolander, Sean Eddy, and Steven Brenner.  Each method laid out in these tools and in other papers had its own flavors and I continued to explore various approaches and applications to phylogeny driven functional prediction.  Examples of my subsequent work are listed below (with links to the Mendeley pages for these papers):

Plus we (at TIGR) used phylogenetic analysis as a tool in annotation of many many genomes as well as metagenomes.

Anyway, enough of history for a bit.  What is interesting about this new paper is that they take a slightly different approach to phylogeny driven functional prediction in that they make use of Gene Ontology functional annotations as their key parameter to trace on evolutionary trees.  They lay out the differences in their method quite well in the introduction:

Our general approach is similar to the ‘phylogenomic’ method proposed by Eisen [6] and further developed into a probabilistic form by Engelhardt et al. [7], but with important differences. Eisen proposed a conceptual approach for predicting protein function using a phylogenetic tree together with available experimental knowledge of proteins. The original approach relied on manual curation to identify gene duplication events and to find and assimilate the literature for characterized members of the family. Engelhardt et al. used automated reconciliation with the species tree [8] to identify gene duplication events, and experimental GO terms (MF only) to capture the experimental literature. Using this information, they defined a probabilistic model of evolution of MF involving transitions between different molecular functions.

From these previous studies, we adopt the basic approach of function evolution through a phylogenetic tree and the use of GO annotations to represent function. However, unlike these other phylogenomic methods, we represent the evolution in terms of discrete gain and loss events. In Eisen’s original model, an annotation does not necessarily represent a gain of function (it could have been inherited from an earlier ancestor), and losses are not explicitly annotated. The transition-based model of Engelhardt et al. assumes replacement of one function by another (gain of one function coupled to the loss of another), and does not capture uncoupled events, which is particularly important for BP annotations and cases where a protein has multiple molecular functions (see examples below). In addition, we make no a priori assumptions about conservation of function within versus between orthologous groups, or about the relationship between evolutionary distance and functional conservation (as the distance may not necessarily reflect every given function). While, as described below, gene duplication events and relatively long tree branches are important clues for curators to locate functional divergence (gain and/or loss), in our paradigm an ancestral function can be inherited by both descendants following a duplication (resulting in paralogs with the same function) or gained/lost by one descendant following a speciation event (resulting in orthologs with different functions). Evolution of each function is evaluated on a case-by-case basis, using many different sources of information about a given protein family

I note – Paul Thomas, one of the authors here has also been developing phylogeny driven functional prediction methods for many years and has done some cool things previously.  This new approach seems novel and useful and their paper is worth looking at.  I like too that they focus on MutS homologs for some of their examples:

Anyway – their paper is worth a read and some of their software tools may be of use including PAINT: http://sourceforge.net/projects/pantherdb/ and http://pantree.org

Good to see continuous developments in phylogeny driven functional predictions.  If you want to learn more – check out the Mendeley Group I have created:

http://www.mendeley.com/groups/1190191/_/widget/29/5/

And please contribute to it. Below are some previous posts of mine of possible interest:

Some links on "ortholog conjecture" paper and critiques of it

Recently a paper by Matt Hahn was published in PLoS Computational Biology entitled “Testing the ortholog conjecture with comparative functional genomic data from mammals.”  The paper created a bit of a stir as some aspects of it call into question some of the standard assumptions made in comparative genomic analysis.

I alas do not have time to go into all the details but fortunately others have tackled this and I am posting some links here:

http://friendfeed.com/erickmatsen/f90bd2c6/emergentnexus-i-think-what-you-were-talking?embed=1

Will try to post my own comments soon.  I note – I am skeptical of their conclusions but still going through the paper to understand everything before commenting in more detail.

Nice review/commentary on challenges in phylogenomic analysis in PLoS Bio by Philippe et al. #fb

This one is definitely worth a read for phylogeneticists and phylogenomicists (is that a word?) out there: PLoS Biology: Resolving Difficult Phylogenetic Questions: Why More Sequences Are Not Enough. Philippe et al. discuss some important issues in using genomes to infer phylogenies of species in this commentary/review paper.  They discuss in particular some recent studies of animal evolution but they cover a lot of useful ground here and include a good review of terminology and some of the basic issues at play.  I am personally going to have to read it in more detail to help deal with some of the issues in our recent study of novel sequences in metagenomic data.

Larry Moran on Phylogenomics, my new paper, and species

Just a quick note to encourage people to check out Larry Moran at The Sandwalk blogging about my new phylogenomics paper (with Martin Wu) and talking about whether one can use species as a term for bacteria.

ASM Phylogenomics Notes

Well, I just gave my talk on phylogenomic and functional predictions and am going to try and catch up with blogging.

In my talk I discussed how an understanding of function and prediction of function requires an understanding of how functions have evolved.  I am trying to get my talk slides posted here but, alas, I need to deal with some Copyright issues first (OK – here is a little slideshow of my talk … no audio sorry)
//www.youtube.com/get_player

Patricia Babbitt gave a talk after mine on another aspect of phylogenomics and functional predictions.  She has done some really interesting studies (see her lab site here) of functional diversifications and the molecular level by integrating genomic, structural, biochemical and phylogenetic analyses.  She showed some really nice tools for clustering and visualization protein families that, although not phylogenetic, seemed to be very useful for the onslaught of genome data.  Unfortunately, most of her publications are not in OA journals so I cannot use any of the figures here and am not going to bother linking to the papers.
Kimmen Sjolander is talking now about her phylogenomic studies.  She is discussion “Phylofacts” which are precomputer phylogenetic trees of gene families from across the tree of life.  One great thing about her work – most of it is published in OA journals and most of her software is available for free download.

More detail on Phylofacts is available in her Genome Biology paper here.  Kimmen has done some really great work on automating phylogenomic functional predictions and this is one example.  Also see her Flower Power and Sci-Phy and Satchmo and Intrepid and Phylo builder other tools (downlaods and other information are available at her website here.)
Gretta Serres will be speaking next.  I will post on here talk soon …  And now I am posting on it …
She is discussing “linking metabolic diversity to protein families” os something to that effect. She discussed something I never have thought of doing which is the following – take genomes, identify the size of protein families in each genome and then cluster genomes by their similarity of the protein family size.  I assume some others must have done this but it seems like a good way to identify similar duplication pressures on distantly related organisms.